Building Executive Function Skills Through Games: The Power of Playful Learning

You are here
The 3- and 4-year-old children in Mr. Garcias classroom are engaged in a game of泭Freeze Dance.
Mr. Garcia says, As the music plays, you may twist, jump, or wiggle, but remember: to keep your body safe, you need to stay a safe distance from泭your neighbor.
The children move along with the music being playeddrums beating a rhythm, horns blowing a spirited melodyand in concert with泭one another.
Remember that when the music stops, says Mr. Garcia, you泭stop too!
As the music continues, the children dance, jump, and share their excitement with泭each other.
Im turning around and around and around,泭says Emmanuel.
When will the music stop?!泭asks Josiah.
Teresa shouts, Look how high I泭can jump!
Madison begins to wander toward the block center but turns her attention back to the game as Mr. Garcia, preparing to pause the music, says, Who remembers what to do when the泭music stops?
The music suddenly stops. The children gasp with energy. Some tumble to the ground, and others freeze with one leg akimbo, arms outstretched. They stop in various stages of movement as the rule of the game comes back to their minds: when the music stops, I泭stop too!
It is easy to see how much care goes into preparing early childhood education classrooms for learning. Understanding that [c]hildren are born eager to learn; [taking] delight exploring their world and making connections (51勛圖厙 2020, 1), teachers like Mr. Garcia create rich environments brimming with signs and activities bolstering a range of skills and knowledge critical for development and learning during the early childhood years and beyond. These are necessary skills for young children entering kindergarten (Sabol & Pianta 2017) and for long-term learning outcomes, such as attaining higher grades and graduating from high school (Duncan et al. 2007; Quirk, Nylund-Gibson, & Furlong 2013; Pan et al. 2019; Ricciardi et泭al. 2021).
Such outcomes do not occur without intention: they are spurred by carefully planned opportunities and experiences within high-quality early childhood education settings (Reid et al. 2021). In high-quality settings, early childhood educators recognize that play promotes joyful learning that fosters self-regulation, language, cognitive and social competencies as well as content knowledge across disciplines (51勛圖厙 2020, 9; see also Yogman et al. 2018). Early childhood educators create, support, and guide childrens learning along a continuum of play experiences. In this way, they help children make progress toward and attain learning goals (Zosh et al. 2022). Examples of these play experiences include games like Freeze Dance, hopscotch, and泭Simon Says.
Indeed, the science of learning shows that children learn best when they are actively engaged in meaningful, iterative, and joyful experiences (Hirsh-Pasek泭et al. 2022; Zosh et al. 2022; Blinkoff, Nesbitt et al. 2023; Nesbitt et al. 2023) that support child agency, a critical piece for creating equitable learning experiences (51勛圖厙 2019; Blinkoff, Wright et al. 2023). Effective early childhood educators intentionally design, select materials, and use a repertoire of strategies so that every child acquires key skills and knowledge through playful lessons and activities. They know that developing these skills takes time, practice, and scaffolding as well as working with others. Interacting and learning with others can be taxing, sometimes manifesting as frustration, resistance, and/or other expressions that may present challenges to educators and peers (Taylor & Boyer 2020). Therefore, teachers purposefully plan playful experiences that develop the simple and more complex skills that help children carry out goal-directed behavior; that is, executive泭function skills.
As a developmental psychologist (Molly, second author) and a former early childhood education teacher and current researcher (Cynthia, first author), we believe that if research indicates these skills are important, then the research community must effectively communicate what is understood. That communication should include practical and playful examples that promote and encourage childrens learning of these skills in early childhood education spaces. The good news is that there are plentiful ways to incorporate play-based activities aimed at building executive function skills in these settings. All of these activities can also be shared with families as teachers partner with them to provide learning opportunities for泭their children.
In this article, we define executive function (including working memory, inhibitory control, and cognitive flexibility) and describe why it is important for childrens learning and development. We explore the connections between executive function and playful learning and offer ways for teachers to support the development of executive function skills through games. Our goal is to continue to encourage early childhood educators in their work, recognizing how important and valuable they are. While the games we share are purposefully easy to incorporate, we recognize that it takes a skillful teacher to carry out the planning, engagement, and gentle encouragement and scaffolding needed as children practice and grow in executive泭function skills.
Executive Function: An泭Evolving Understanding
The concept of executive functionand its importance for learning and developmenthas been given increasing attention in recent years (Moreno, Shwayder, & Friedman 2016; Ackerman &泭Friedman-Krauss泭2017; Gibb et al. 2021). However, researchers have not come to consensus about how these constructs are defined and how skills are delineated from each other, measured, and connected to practice. Recently, there have been efforts to clarify terms and solidify shared understandings (Jones et al. 2016). In this section, we present definitions and explanations of executive function that are grounded in泭these efforts.
Executive function has been characterized as the brains air traffic control system (Center on the Developing Child, n.d.; Shonkoff et al. 2011).泭Just as air traffic control directs the paths of airplanes by helping them successfully take off, move, and land in destinations around the globe, executive function skills help individuals move from place to place, complete tasks, and stay focused. In early childhood education settings in particular, these skills keep children safe and the classroom community泭moving smoothly.
Executive function is comprised of the individual skills of泭working memory,泭inhibitory control, and泭cognitive flexibility泭(Center on the Developing泭Child, n.d.).
- Working memory泭allows children to hold and update information over short periods of time. Children are asked to remember an incredible amount of information during the course of the school day. For example, in the opening vignette, Mr. Garcia asked the children to remember the steps needed to play Freeze Dance and to follow rules to keep themselves and others safe. The children worked hard to hold the multiple directions of movement and safety in mind, all while working toward the goal of stopping when the泭music stopped.
- Inhibitory control泭allows children to suppress behavior or responses. It is not uncommon for children to veer off task as they juggle the multiple instructions they were asked to keep in mind while playing a game, for instance. A vibrant early childhood setting may be filled with wonderful and age-appropriate distractions (the other children, toys, childrens own desires), all serving to make keeping on task challenging. For example, in the opening vignette, Madison had to practice inhibitory control to move her attention from the block center back to the game. As children grow and develop, they become better at remaining focused on a task and at ignoring other appealing, or even disruptive, distractions that may interfere with reaching泭their goal.
- Cognitive flexibility泭allows children to shift attention between competing priorities and to think in new ways. For example, the children in the opening vignette may have been encouraged, on the one hand, to move individually in creative ways. But, on the other hand, dancing safely in a group requires moving without infringing on other children, whether physically or emotionally. So they had to think about competing priorities to balance their own creative movements with their own and others safety. The children had to think about this game and its rules and actions in a泭new way.
For an overview of how brain science research informs our understanding of these skills, see Evidence from Neuroscience Research: What We Know About Executive Function and the Developing Brain below.
The Importance of Executive Function Skills for泭Childrens Learning
Classroom research tells us executive function helps contribute to childrens academic and social and emotional learning (Shaul & Schwartz 2014). For example, preschoolers who have better inhibitory control are rated as having higher social and emotional competence by teachers (Rhoades, Greenberg, & Domitrovich 2009). Studies show that executive function, or the individual skills that make up executive function, are associated with content area learning and success, including in math and literacy (Blair & Razza 2007; Fuhs et al. 2014). Moreover, Nesbitt and colleagues (2015) probed this association and found that children with higher executive function ability were more engaged in positive learning opportunities and less disruptive in the classroom: these泭learning-related泭behaviors resulted in larger math and literacy gains. We understand this to mean that these skills (the academic and the regulatory) develop in tandem (Blair &泭Razza 2007).
Executive function skills are learned in the workings of early childhood settings over time, with practice and intentional teaching. Early childhood educators understand that children may, for example, have difficulty paying attention to a circle time discussion when they cannot keep the conversation in mind, are distracted, or behave in ways that challenge the circle time setting (talking or walking away). Further, no two young children in early childhood settings will be the same. Children need varying amounts of time and opportunities to develop, and they need to be supported in developmentally appropriate and equitable ways. Evidence shows that executive function skills consistently develop well into adolescence and adulthood (Gogtay et al. 2004; Best, Miller, & Jones 2009; Souissi, Chamari, & Bellaj 2022). Childrens development of executive function is an incremental and lengthy process that depends on a variety of factors, including the adults around them who continue to practice and develop regulation-related skills themselves (Doebel 2020; Zelazo &泭Carlson 2020).
Evidence from Neuroscience Research: What We Know About Executive Function and the泭Developing Brain
Neuroscience research shows that the prefrontal cortex, the brain region responsible for executive function, develops rapidly in childhood (M羹ller & Kerns 2015) and is not fully developed until well into adulthood (Gogtay et al. 2004; Gilmore, Knickmeyer, & Gao. 2018; Bethlehem et al. 2022). Researchers can witness this maturation by assessing childrens adaptability to a changing rule in a card game, for instance. Although 3-year-old children are often unable to adapt to a change in the rule, 5-year-old children usually can (Zelazo 2006). Brain science describes a link between the development of the frontal lobe of the brain, the area responsible for executive function, and the amygdala, an area of the brain partially responsible for regulation (Silvers et al. 2017). As educators have likely observed, this research tells us that the older children are, the better they are at managing their own behavior, especially when they泭are upset.
The iterative reprocessing model posits that a childs development of executive function skills rests on the neurocognitive skill of泭reflection, or the ability to step back and deliberately sustain attention on泭goal-directed泭problem solving (Zelazo 2015). For a child to increase their executive function skills, their ability to reflect must also be amplified. The more the brain practices reflection, the more executive function skills can improve. Indeed, when the brain is used in a certain way time and time again, those neural circuits become more efficient. For instance, Diamond (2013) highlights the developmental progression of the components of executive function. Repeated practice is key to fostering these skills in泭educational settings.
In addition, brain development, including the development of executive function, is impacted by a childs environment. Unfortunately, some children may face toxic stress, or stress that is prolonged and harmful, stemming from experiences like poverty or abuse (Franke 2014). Evidence shows that toxic stress can have lifelong, deleterious impacts on childrens brain development, including on executive function ability (Shonkoff et al. 2012; Shonkoff 2017). Teachers are likely aware that not all environments provide children with equal opportunities to build executive function skills. Some children face significant issues and events, whether one time or ongoing, that impact their overall development and learning (Georgieff, Ramel, & Cusick 2018;泭Horowitz-Kraus泭& Hutton 2018; Tooley, Bassett, &泭Mackey 2021).
Some key takeaways from brain science泭research include:
- Particular areas of the brain responsible for executive function and self-regulation develop over time. The younger a child is, the more challenging it will be for them to implement executive function泭and self-regulation.
- As children grow, they become better at these important skills, often moving from developing simpler skills to more泭complex ones.
- High-quality early learning environments and experiences foster泭childrens development.
- There is variation among children, which depends on individual characteristics泭and contexts.
- Childrens exposure to environmental toxic stress can impact the development of泭executive function.
泭
Games: Playful Experiences to Promote泭Executive Function
Playful learning contributes to the development of executive function skills. Games are one type of playful learning experience that can be planned for and guided by teachers. Once children learn a specific game, they can play it independently, giving teachers the opportunity to observe and assess childrens executive function skills. For example, Simon Says targets the elements of executive function that link to academic and social and emotional learning, all while engaging children in a playful experience. It can also be adapted to give children agency by letting them be Simon, thinking of actions for their peers. The rules can be changed to provide a challenge for children: once children are sufficiently experienced playing Simon Says in the traditional way, teachers can ask them to泭not泭do what Simon says.
Playful moments that help children develop executive function skills can also be integrated into transition times. Cleaning up is a great opportunity to practice sorting, for instance. Teachers can encourage children to sort items such as blocks by color as they place them back on a shelf or in bins. To help children build cognitive flexibility, teachers can go back and forth between different sorting rules, combining rules (sorting by color, shape, and texture) for an泭extra challenge.
In this section, we offer vignettes showing early childhood educators and children engaged in examples of games that support executive function skillssuch as memory card games and泭Jumping Feet.
Memory泭Card Games
Ms. Samuelson invites the children in her class to help create a set of cards for a memory game. She encourages the children to take photos of one another, giving them the choice of how and when to take the images. She prints two copies of each photo and glues them onto cardstock or posterboard so that the children can easily manipulate泭the cards.
After the cards have been created, Ms. Samuelson spreads them out on the table and invites children to泭join her.
Were going to play memory. Who remembers the rules?泭she asks.
We turn over two cards,泭says Jeffery.
Yes, you rememberedwe take turns turning over two cards at a time. And what do we do if the泭cards match?
The children reply excitedly, Its a pair. We泭keep them!
What happens if the cards泭dont泭match? asks泭Ms. Samuelson.
Katie replies, Janie gets泭a turn.
Yes, says Ms. Samuelson. If the cards dont match, remember to turn them back over, and the next classmate will take泭their turn.
In small groups, children can develop both their collaboration and executive function skills by playing a memory card game. This game helps children develop working memory skills. Teachers can create a set of cards that depict objects of different colors, shapes, textures, sizes, or other dimensionsor add interest by including images of familiar objects from the classroom (the block shelf, the water pitchers used at snack time, the bins of crayons or buttons). Teachers can also support children as they make a set of cards, like Ms. Samuelson did, giving them ownership in the games creation. No matter what images are used, the rules can be adapted to target more than one executive function skill. For example, matching images by shape泭and泭size helps children practice both working memory and泭cognitive flexibility.
Jumping Feet
Mr. Robert notices a group of children looking to play while outside. He decides to introduce them to a new game called Jumping Feet. Removing chalk from his pocket, he lowers himself to the playgrounds textured surface, asking All right, who knows how to泭play hopscotch?
Arms fly up as the children shout, I do! and Me, me,泭me, me!
Great, says Mr. Robert, Playing Jumping Feet泭is a lot like hopscotch. See here on the ground where Ive drawn some feet? As you look, you may see two feet or you may see one foot. This game is like hopscotch, but theres a fun twist in it. When you see two feet on our path, you jump with two feet. But when you see one foot, you jump with one foot. Who would like to泭go first?
After the children have some experience playing Jumping Feet this way, Mr. Robert changes泭the rules.
Instead of using two feet to jump when you see two feet, use one foot. When you see one foot, use泭two feet.
Darla begins her turn but forgets the new rules and jumps with two feet onto the outline of two feet. She frowns, looking to Mr. Robert for encouragement泭and guidance.
This is hard to do. But dont worry. With practice, we can do it, says Mr. Robert. Lets try it again! When you see two feet this time, use just one泭foot there.
Darla begins another turn and jumps with one foot on that outline. Turns continue, and each child has a chance to try this new twist on泭the game.
Young children love to move, and Jumping Feet offers them an opportunity to practice moving their bodies as well as executive function and self-regulation skills. Like Mr. Robert in the vignette, teachers can begin guiding children to play this game in the traditional sense (children jump on the picture of feet they see by using the number of feet they see). After applying that set of rules, Mr. Robert challenged the children by changing the rules. This supports the development of cognitive flexibility and working memory as children must keep the new rule in mind. In addition, children engaged in inhibitory control. Instead of immediately acting based on the familiar rules, they had to inhibit their movements and actions: they had to pause to consider the number of feet they saw on the path and then do the opposite.泭This is hard work for children and so important for their continued development. Over time, children will gradually and more easily employ these skills later and in泭different settings.
These are just a sampling of games that educators can integrate into their settings. For more ideas of playful ways to support executive function, see Harvard Universitys Center on the Developing Child () and the Playful Learning Landscape Action泭Network ().
Conclusion
Among other benefits, playful learning fosters executive function skills, which are important foundations for learning and development. We encourage early childhood educators, administrators, and families to raise their voices in support of the power of play in all aspects of learning and in support of the right of children to be engaged in playful ways that elicit joy and tap into opportunities of tremendous growth and unlimited potential. We wish all readers a classroom full of playful泭learning experiences!
Photographs: 穢 Getty Images
Copyright 穢 2024 by the 51勛圖厙. See Permissions and Reprints online at 51勛圖厙.org/resources/permissions.
References
Ackerman, D.J., & A.H. Friedman-Krauss. 2017. Preschoolers Executive Function: Importance, Contributors, Research Needs and Assessment Options.泭ETS Research Report Series泭1: 124.
Best, J.R., P.H. Miller, & L.L. Jones. 2009. Executive Functions After Age 5: Changes and Correlates.泭Developmental Review泭29 (3): 180200.
Bethlehem, R.A.I., J. Seidlitz, S.R. White, J.W. Vogel, K.M. Anderson, et al. 2022. Brain Charts for the Human Lifespan.泭Nature泭604 (7906): 52533.
Blair, C., & R.P. Razza. 2007. Relating Effortful Control, Executive Function, and False Belief Understanding to Emerging Math and Literacy Ability in Kindergarten.泭Child Development泭78 (2): 64763.
Blinkoff, E., K.T. Nesbitt, R.M. Golinkoff, & K. Hirsh-Pasek. 2023. Investigating the Contributions of Active, Playful Learning to Student Interest and Educational Outcomes.泭Acta Psychologica泭238: 103983.
Blinkoff, E., C.A. Wright, M. Scott, K. Fletcher, A.S. Masters, et al. 2023. Shifting from a Classroom of Reluctant Compliance to a Classroom of Responsive Curiosity.泭Young Children泭78 (3): 1422.
Center on the Developing Child. n.d. Executive Function & Self-Regulation. Accessed July 8, 2023. .
Diamond, A. 2013. Executive Functions.泭Annual Review of Psychology泭64 (1): 13568.
Doebel, S. 2020. Rethinking Executive Function and Its Development.泭Perspectives on Psychological Science泭15 (4): 94256.
Duncan, G.J., C.J. Dowsett, A. Claessens, K. Magnuson, A.C. Huston, et al. 2007. School Readiness and Later Achievement.泭Developmental Psychology泭43 (6): 142846.
Franke, H.A. 2014. Toxic Stress: Effects, Prevention and Treatment.泭Children泭1 (3): 390402.
Fuhs, M.W., K.T. Nesbitt, D.C. Farran, & N. Dong. 2014. Longitudinal Associations Between Executive Functioning and Academic Skills Across Content Areas.泭Developmental Psychology泭50 (6): 16981709.
Georgieff, M.K., S.E. Ramel, & S.E. Cusick. 2018. Nutritional Influences on Brain Development.泭Acta Paediatrica泭107 (8): 131021.
Gibb, R., L. Coelho, N.A. Van Rootselaar, C. Halliwell, M. MacKinnon, et al. 2021. Promoting Executive Function Skills in Preschoolers Using a Play-Based Program.泭Frontiers in泭Psychology泭12: 720225.
Gilmore, J.H., R.C. Knickmeyer, & W. Gao. 2018. Imaging Structural and Functional Brain Development in Early Childhood.泭Nature Reviews Neuroscience泭19 (3): 12337.
Gogtay, N., J.N. Giedd, L. Lusk, K.M. Hayashi, D. Greenstein, et al. 2004. Dynamic Mapping of Human Cortical Development During Childhood Through Early Adulthood.泭Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences泭101 (21): 817479.
Hirsh-Pasek, K., R.M. Golinkoff, K. Nesbitt, C. Lautenbach, E. Blinkoff, et al. 2022.泭Making Schools Work: Bringing the Science of Learning to Joyful Classroom Practice. New York, NY: Teachers College Press.
Horowitz-Kraus, T., & J.S. Hutton. 2018. Brain Connectivity in Children Is Increased by the Time They Spend Reading Books and Decreased by the Length of Exposure to Screen-Based Media.泭Acta Paediatrica泭107 (4): 68593.
Jones, S.M., R. Bailey, S.P. Barnes, & A. Partee. 2016.泭Executive Function Mapping Project: Untangling the Terms and Skills Related to Executive Function and Self-Regulation in Early Childhood.泭OPRE Report #2016-88. Washington, DC: Office of Planning, Research and Evaluation, Administration for Children and Families, US Department of Health and Human Services. .
Moreno, A.J., I. Shwayder, & I.D. Friedman. 2016. The Function of Executive Function: Everyday Manifestations of Regulated Thinking in Preschool Settings.泭Early Childhood Education Journal泭45 (2): 14353.
M羹ller, U., & K. Kerns. 2015. The Development of Executive Function. In泭Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science: Cognitive Processes, eds. L.S. Liben, U. M羹ller, & R.M. Lerner, 571623. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
51勛圖厙. 2019. Advancing Equity in Early Childhood Education.泭Position statement. Washington, DC: 51勛圖厙. 51勛圖厙.org/resources/position-statements/equity.
51勛圖厙. 2020. Developmentally Appropriate Practice. Position statement. Washington, DC: 51勛圖厙. 51勛圖厙.org/resources/position-statements/dap/contents.
Nesbitt, K.T., E. Blinkoff, R.M. Golinkoff, & K. Hirsh-Pasek. 2023. Making Schools Work: An Equation for Active Playful Learning.泭Theory into Practice泭62 (2): 14154.
Nesbitt, K.T., D.C. Farran, & M.G. Fuhs. 2015. Executive Function Skills and Academic Achievement Gains in Prekindergarten: Contributions of Learning-Related Behaviors.泭Developmental Psychology泭51 (7): 86578.
Pan, Q., K.T. Trang, H.R. Love, & J. Templin. 2019. School Readiness Profiles and Growth in Academic Achievement.泭Frontiers in Education泭4: 127.
Quirk, M., K. Nylund-Gibson, & M. Furlong. 2013. Exploring Patterns of Latino/a Childrens School Readiness at Kindergarten Entry and Their Relations with Grade 2 Achievement.泭Early Childhood Research Quarterly泭28 (2): 43749.
Reid, J.L., S.L. Kagan, J. Brooks-Gunn, & S.A. Melvin. 2021. Promoting Quality in Programs for Infants and Toddlers: Comparing the Family Child Care and Center-Based Teaching Workforce.泭Children and Youth Services Review泭122: 105890.
Rhoades, B.L., M.T. Greenberg, & C.E. Domitrovich. 2009. The Contribution of Inhibitory Control to Preschoolers SocialEmotional Competence.泭Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology泭30 (3): 31020.
Ricciardi, C., M. Louis, S. Hartman, C. Bleiker, L. Dineheart, et al. 2021. School Readiness Skills at Age Four Predict Academic Achievement Through 5th Grade.泭Early Childhood Research Quarterly泭57: 11020.
Sabol, T.J., & R.C. Pianta. 2017. The State of Young Children in the United States: School Readiness. In泭The Wiley Handbook of Early Childhood Development Programs, Practices, and Policies, eds. E. Votruba-Drzal & E. Dearing, 117. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Shaul, S., & M. Schwartz. 2014. The Role of the Executive Functions in School Readiness Among Preschool-Age Children.泭Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal泭27 (4): 74968.
Shonkoff, J.P. 2017. Breakthrough Impacts: What Science Tells Us About Supporting Early Childhood Development.泭Young Children泭72 (2): 816.
Shonkoff, J.P., A.S. Garner, Committee on Psychosocial Aspects of Child and Family Health, Committee on Early Childhood, Adoption, and Dependent Care, and Section on Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, B. S. Siegel, M.I. Dobbins, M.F. Earls, et al. 2012. The Lifelong Effects of Early Childhood Adversity and Toxic Stress.泭Pediatrics泭129 (1): 23246.
Shonkoff, J.P., G.J. Duncan, P.A. Fisher, K. Magnuson, & C. Raver. 2011. Building the Brains Air Traffic Control System: How Early Experiences Shape the Development of Executive Function.泭National Scientific Council on the Developing Child泭11: 118.
Silvers, J.A., C. Insel, A. Powers, P. Franz, C. Helion, et al. 2017. vlPFCvmPFCAmygdala Interactions Underlie Age-Related Differences in Cognitive Regulation of Emotion.泭Cerebral Cortex泭27 (7): 350214.
Souissi, S., K. Chamari, & T. Bellaj. 2022. Assessment of Executive Functions in School-Aged Children: A Narrative Review.泭Frontiers in Psychology泭13: 991699.
Taylor, M.E., & W. Boyer. 2020. Play-Based Learning: Evidence-Based Research to Improve Childrens Learning Experiences in the Kindergarten Classroom.泭Early Childhood Education Journal泭48 (2): 12733.
Tooley, U.A., D.S. Bassett, & A.P. Mackey. 2021. Environmental Influences on the Pace of Brain Development.泭Nature Reviews Neuroscience泭22 (6): 37284.
Yogman, M., A. Garner, J. Hutchinson, K. Hirsh-Pasek, & R.M. Golinkoff. 2018. The Power of Play: A Pediatric Role in Enhancing Development in Young Children.泭Pediatrics泭142 (3): .
Zelazo, P.D. 2006. The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): A Method of Assessing Executive Function in Children.泭Nature Protocols泭1 (1): 297301.
Zelazo, P.D. 2015. Executive Function: Reflection, Iterative Reprocessing, Complexity, and the Developing Brain.泭Developmental Review泭38 (4): 5568.
Zelazo, P.D., & S.M. Carlson. 2020. The Neurodevelopment of Executive Function Skills: Implications for Academic Achievement Gaps.泭Psychology & Neuroscience泭13 (3): 27398.
Zosh, J.M., C. Gaudreau, R.M. Golinkoff, & K. Hirsh-Pasek. 2022. The Power of Playful Learning in the Early Childhood Setting. In泭Developmentally Appropriate Practice in Early Childhood Programs Serving Children from Birth Through Age 8, 4th ed., eds. S. Friedman, S. Bredekamp, M. Masterson, B. Willer, & B.L. Wright, 613. Washington, DC: 51勛圖厙.
Cynthia A. Wiltshire,泭EdD, is an assistant professor of early childhood education at the University of Texas at El Paso. She is a former classroom teacher and spent 12 of 20 years in the early childhood context. Dr. Wiltshires research focuses on early childhood education workforce well-being. [email protected]
Molly Scott,泭PhD, is the associate director of the Active Playful Learning project, a study examining the impact of an approach to teaching that emphasizes active, engaged, and meaningful learning that is collaborative, iterative, and joyful. [email protected]